Example of Research Journal Assignment
Scientific Journal Report + Article
Ashley A. Adams
December 6, 2005
1. What is the name of your journal?
Psychology of Women Quarterly.
- For whom does it seem to have been written? For example, is it directed toward a special kind of psychologist? Does it include articles that would be of interest to educators or others outside the field of academic psychology? If so, list several titles.
The journal seems to be geared to psychologists with a feminist approach and who concentrate mainly on female based studies. Its articles would have an appeal to an audience beyond the world of psychology specifically the feminist collegiate audience.***
- Choose a representative research article whose title interests you. Write the name of its title and briefly explain why it interests you.
“If only I were thin like her, maybe I could be happy like her”: The self-implications of associating a thin female ideal with life success. This is article is of interest to me because I often find that I compare myself to thin-ideal media images, and the more I do so the less confident I find myself that day.
- How long is the article?
Six pages.
- List the major sections of the article as defined by the heads.
The major sections are: title, summary, introduction, method, discussion, results, and references.
- Does the author state the hypothesis of the research study? Write the hypothesis in the author’s words.
The author states the hypothesis as: “It is hypothesized that when life success stereotypes about a thin-ideal female target are explicitly challenged or negated, women should report more positive self-perceptions than when these life success stereotypes are explicitly or implicitly confirmed.”
- Write the hypothesis in your own words.
When a negative scenario about an ideally thin woman is presented, the women studied should feel better about themselves than when presented with a positive scenario pertaining to the ideally thin woman.
- Who made up the study population?
The study consisted of 126 women in introductory level psychology classes at a large Midwestern university. The women are mainly European American (83.3%) aging from 17 to 42 with body mass indexes ranging from 16 to 60.
- Does the article contain a section on the method used in conducting the study? Describe the method.
Yes. All participants were presented with one of two photos of ideally thin women and randomly assigned to read one of four passages regarding the life-satisfaction of the target in the photo. They were then asked to complete several different surveys including the life-outcomes questionnaire, the mood measure, and the state self-esteem measure.
- Which of the methods of scientific research described in Chapter 1 is used?
This would be considered an experiment which uses several surveys to assess its subjects.
- Is there a discussion of the significance of results?
Yes.
- Summarize the significance of the results in your own words.
The results show that women who received stereotype-confirming information about the thin-ideal target reported less optimistic future life outcomes, less positive moods, and lower appearance and social state self-esteem than did those women who were presented with stereotype-disconfirming information. These same groups did not, however, report significant differences on negative life outcomes or performance self-esteem.
- What conclusions are reached in your article?
The results suggest that body image may not be all that causes self-dissatisfaction when women are exposed to thin images. The social context related to the thin model may also play heavily in these feeling of dissatisfaction. Women’s motivation to attain an ideally thin figure may go beyond physical appearance and pertain just as much to attaining life satisfaction. It is concluded that research based solely on body-dissatisfaction may not encompass all that makes up our understanding of physical appearance and that “body-dissatisfaction take place within a social and cultural context.” (Evans 213)
- What is your reaction to the research article? For example, were there sections that you found difficult to understand? Were there section that seemed very “scientific?” Are you convinced of the conclusions? Why or why not?
This article brought to light a lot about what motivates me to attain the ideal figure, and why I get so discouraged after spending a few hours flipping through fashion and celebrity magazines. The method used for the experiment made sense to me and I found the use of four different scenarios sensible. I did think that it was rather simplistic to think that the results of the 3 different surveys could all be affected solely by the scenarios read, rather than what might be going on the immediate live of the participants. The technical information found in the results section highlighting the numerical data obtained from the surveys was somewhat lost on me, as I was not familiar with the formulas used for such data compilation. I found this frustrating since I do hold a degree in mathematics and usually find the numbers fascinating when I understand exactly how they were obtained. I was especially puzzled by the statement under the heading Body Mass Index in regards to conducting the analyses of the data “with participants’ BMI scores covaried out.” I wasn’t sure what this meant exactly, and wasn’t entirely sure that BMI wouldn’t have played an important role in how the different women interpreted what they read and saw.
- Summarize the article in your own words. How did it benefit you and how might it benefit us if we were to read it?
The article explores the possibility that women not only view ideally thin female media images with the desire to obtain that image for physical self-satisfaction but also for obtaining better life satisfaction. The author conducted an experiment with a large group of college women to determine if the social context surrounding the target image had an effect on the subject’s life outlook. It was determined that disconfirming the stereotype that thin-ideal women lead more satisfying lives resulted in higher self-satisfaction of the subjects. Results prove that there is more to women’s desire to obtain an ideally thin physique than appearances alone, but also in overall life satisfaction. Reading this article helped to open my eyes to the way that viewing thin-ideal media images affects my own self-esteem and outlook on life. I don’t think that my self-satisfaction is ruled by these images, but I do believe that I spend an inordinate amount of my time thinking about a thinner prettier me. This article would probably benefit most women in being able to enlighten them as to how these images can affect more that just physical self-satisfaction. I believe this article has real cultural implications regarding the over abundance of unrealistically thin female body images in the media and the effect it has on the average female’s self evaluation.
*Please excuse the formatting of this document. I had some difficulty with pasting a .PDF file into a word document. This is my best attempt to recreate the original formatting of the document. The page numbers were removed in order to give the article better flow in this word document. The article appeared on pages 209 -214 of the Psychology of Women Quarterly.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27 (2003), 209–214. Blackwell Publishing. Printed in the USA.
Copyright C_ 2003 Division 35, American Psychological Association. 0361-6843/03
“IF ONLY I WERE THIN LIKE HER, MAYBE I COULD BE
HAPPY LIKE HER”: THE SELF-IMPLICATIONS OF
ASSOCIATING A THIN FEMALE IDEAL WITH LIFE SUCCESS
Peggy Chin Evans
Michigan State University
Women often feel dissatisfied with their appearance after comparing themselves to other females who epitomize the
thin-ideal standard of beauty. The current study posits that women associate a thin-ideal female body type with positive
life-success, and that it may be this psychological link that drives feelings of negativity toward the self after such upward
social comparisons. The results revealed that women reported more self-dissatisfaction and less optimism about their
possible future life outcomes after exposure to a thin-ideal female target that ostensibly had a successful life than when
the target ostensibly had an unsuccessful life.
Research indicates that while the ideal standard of beauty
for females commonly portrayed in the media has become
thinner than it had been in the past (Silverstein,
Perdue, Peterson, & Kelly, 1986), the average body size
of adult North American females has increased (Spitzer,
Henderson, & Zivian, 1999). Not surprisingly, North American
women often report being dissatisfied with their bodies
and with their weight in particular, especially after exposure
to Western media images that display a thin-ideal standard
of beauty (e.g., Cattarin, Thompson, Thomas, & Williams,
2000; Thornton&Moore, 1993). These studies suggest that
many North American women make upward social comparisons
to ideal beauty standards in the media to assess
their level of attractiveness and to evaluate their own appearance.
However, a number of studies have shown that
upward social comparisons to ideal standards of beauty can
make women feel negatively about themselves (Thornton
& Maurice, 1997, 1999).
A recent meta-analysis which examined the effects
of the mass media on female body image revealed that
women reported feeling significantly worse after exposure
to thin-ideal media images than after viewing average-sized
or plus-sized media images (Groesz, Levine, & Murnen,
2002). Similarly, a study on social comparison, body image,
This research was conducted in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the author’s doctoral degree at Michigan State University.
The author thanks the three anonymous reviewers who
provided helpful comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Peggy Chin
Evans. E-mail: peggy@chin-evans.com
and the media showed that women who engaged in social
comparisons with thin-ideal female media images in
appearance-related commercials reported substantially
more anger, anxiety, and depression than women who
saw a non-appearance related commercial (Cattarin et al.,
2000). Taken together, these studies suggest that women
are encouraged by the media to compare themselves to
unrealistic thin-ideal standards of bodily attractiveness
(Silverstein et al., 1986), which may then lead to their
body dissatisfaction (see Myers & Biocca, 1992, for an
alternative interpretation). Although conventional wisdom
would suggest that women compare themselves to and
want to emulate the physiques of thin-ideal standards,
which, in turn, affects their perceptions of their own
bodies, this assumption may not be entirely correct.
It is possible that women strive for the thin-ideal body
type by associating thinness with positive life success, and
it may be this life success that women strive to achieve via
having a thin-ideal body.Astudy by Harrison (1997) demonstrated
a positive relationship between women’s attraction
to thin female television characters (operationalized as liking,
feeling similar to, and wanting to be like the character)
and a personal desire for thinness. This study suggests that
thinness alone may not be sufficient to explain why women
are motivated to emulate the physiques of female television
personalities. Rather, women’s desire to become more like
the thin image in terms of the character’s projected lifestyle
and personality may prompt them to strive for these thinideal
standards.
Women may feel dissatisfied with themselves after engaging
in comparisons with thin media ideals because such
standards induce women to imagine an alternative world
where they could lead very different lives if their physical
appearance were altered. Thus, the possible selves
that women generate for themselves may influence feelings
about the current self. The concept of possible selves
is defined as the elements of one’s self-concept that represent
a sense of what one might become, what one would like
to become, and what one is afraid of becoming (Markus &
Nurius, 1986). Previous studies indicate that peoples’ possible
selves are susceptible to change depending on the current
environment and may be sensitive to information that
conveys new or inconsistent “data” about the self (Markus
& Nurius, 1986). For example, a woman who is exposed
to a photograph of a thin, attractive female may feel less
attractive and expect her future possible selves to be more
negative than before seeing the photograph. Hence, how
an individual feels about her possible life-outcomes may
depend on the stimulus to which she is exposed.
The purpose of the current study is to examine how
life success stereotypes about thin-ideal females can influence
women’s self-perceptions after a social comparison to
an ideal figure. It is hypothesized that when life success
stereotypes about a thin-ideal female target are explicitly
challenged or negated, women should report more positive
self-perceptions than when these life success stereotypes
are explicitly or implicitly confirmed. The affirmation
of this hypothesis would suggest that it is more than just
body dissatisfaction that women experience after exposure
to thin-media ideals. Rather, it is also the belief that women
with ideal bodies also have ideal lives, and it may be the ideal
life that women strive to attain via having a more perfect
body.
METHOD
Participants
Participants were 126 women recruited from introductorylevel
psychology classes at a large Midwestern university
in exchange for course-related credit. Of the participants,
83.3% were European American, 11.1% were
African American, 3.2% were Asian American, and the remaining
2.4% were from other ethnic backgrounds. Participants’
ages ranged from 17 to 42 (M = 19.5, SD = 2.6)
and their body mass index (BMI) ranged from 16.1 to 60.0
(M = 24.3, SD = 5.9; Healthy BMI scores fall between 18
to 25; Korbonits et al., 1997).
Design and Procedure
Participants were told that this study was about the impact
of the media on health. Upon arriving at the laboratory,
participants were greeted by a female experimenter who
had them complete a consent form and fill out a questionnaire
asking them about their dietary habits, including
how frequently they skipped meals, ate “junk food,”
and exercised. They were also asked whether they considered
themselves to be in “good shape.” In actuality,
participants completed this measure in order to activate
their thoughts about their weight and lifestyle. Following
this, the experimenter weighed the participants in front
of a mirror and measured their height in order to make
their own physical appearance more highly salient to them.
Thoughts about their weight and physical appearance were
activated in order to increase the likelihood that participants
would attend to the physical appearance of the thinideal
targets and the lifestyle information, thereby increasing
the probability that participants would engage in social
comparisons with the thin-ideal target model on those two
dimensions.
All participants viewed one of two photographs of a thin,
attractive, European American female target (pretested for
attractiveness by a separate sample). Participants were then
randomly assigned to one of four conditions. In Condition 1,
participants read a positive-success passage about the target,
which stated that, a nationwide survey of 1,000 thin,
average-weight, and heavier women showed that “by far,
the group with the most life-satisfaction was thin women.”
Additionally, the passage stated that the target in the photograph
was contacted, and she confirmed that she enjoyed a
happy life. In Condition 2, participants read an unsuccessful
passage, which was identical to the first passage but stated,
“by far, the group with the least amount of life-satisfaction
was thin women.” The target further confirmed that she
had an unhappy life. In Condition 3, participants read a
positive-negated passage, which stated that thin women
“do not enjoy more happiness than others, are not more
successful than others, and do not benefit from any special
advantages over others.” Once again, participants were
told that the target confirmed that she had an average life
with normal “ups and downs” just like everybody else. Participants
in Condition 4 were given no information about
the target’s lifestyle. However, considerable research has
shown that thin-ideal females are typically perceived by others
to enjoy positive life success (e.g., Hebl & Heatherton,
1998).
After participants were exposed to the photographs and
lifestyle passages, they completed the life-outcomes questionnaire.
Participants then indicated how they felt at the
moment by completing the mood measure and the state
self-esteem measure (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991). Participants
were fully debriefed about the study at the end of
the lab session. They were told that all of the lifestyle
information that they received was fabricated and that
“in general, most people experience some good things
in their lives and some bad things. There is no group
that has a particularly happy life based on their physical
appearance.”
Measures
Life outcomes. Based on a modified version of the possible
selves questionnaire (Markus & Nurius, 1986), the
life-outcomes measure asked participants to indicate on a
scale ranging from 1 (very unlikely to be true of me) to 9
(very likely to be true of me) the likelihood that they would
experience 32 positive (e.g., “I may be admired by others,”
“I may lead an exciting life”) and negative life circumstances
(e.g., “I may be pitied by others,” “I may be unhealthy”).
These items were pilot-tested in an earlier unrelated study
measuring the likelihood that a variety of situations could be
true of thin and fat women. Participants’ scores on the items
that measured positive life-outcomes were averaged to constitute
the positive life-outcomes variable (alpha = .85) and
participants’ scores on the items that measured negative
life outcomes were averaged to constitute the negative lifeoutcomes
variable (alpha = .86).
Mood. General mood after exposure to stimulus materials
was measured by asking participants to describe
how “thinking about the target in the photograph makes
you feel about yourself right now.” Participants were
asked to make ten bipolar ratings based on 9-point
scales ranging from –4 to +4 on the following adjectives:
depressed-elated, stressed-relaxed, dissatisfied-satisfied,
unhappy-happy, disappointed-relieved, insecure-secure,
self-conscious-self-confident, guilty-innocent, ashamedproud,
and negative-positive. The scores from these ten
adjectives were then averaged to create the mood variable
(alpha = .96).
State self-esteem. Possible momentary changes in selfesteem
after exposure to stimulus materials were measured
using the state self-esteem scale (SSES; Heatherton &
Polivy, 1991). This scale is a 20-item questionnaire with
three components: (a) appearance (6 items, alpha = .85);
(b) social (7 items, alpha = .83); and (c) performance (7
items, alpha = .80). The scale asks participants to reflect
on how they feel about themselves “right now” on items
such as, “I am pleased with my appearance right now”
(appearance), “I feel self-conscious” (social), and “I feel
confident about my abilities” (performance). State selfesteem
is measured by asking participants to rate themselves
on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)
with some items reverse scored (Heatherton & Polivy,
1991).
RESULTS
Comparability of Stimulus Materials
Because this study used two different photographs of female
models, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was
conducted to show that the two models did not elicit different
responses on the dependent variables, F(6, 119) = .62,
ns. Because the two figures in the photographs showed no
significant differences across the dependent variables, the
data for the two photographs depicting the female figures
were collapsed throughout the rest of the analyses.
Body Mass Index
Participants’ actual BMI may influence the degree to which
they feel self-dissatisfied after comparing themselves to a
thin-ideal female image. Thus, all analyses were conducted
with participants’ BMI scores covaried out.
Differences Between Conditions (Stereotype Consistent
vs. Stereotype Inconsistent)
Two conditions (Condition 1 and Condition 4) composed
the stereotype-consistent category, in which Condition
1 explicitly supported the positive life-stereotypes
of thin females and Condition 4 implicitly supported
the stereotype. The other two conditions (Condition 2
and Condition 3) composed the stereotype-inconsistent
category, in which Condition 2 explicitly challenged the
positive life-stereotypes of thin females and Condition 3
negated the stereotype. To explore whether the strength
of manipulation varied within lifestyle information categories,
univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were
conducted to explore the possibility of differences between
Conditions 1 and 4, as well as Conditions 2 and 3, on the
dependent variables. There were no differences between
Conditions 1 and 4 or between Conditions 2 and 3. Of
particular interest to this study, the strength of manipulation
for both the stereotype-consistent conditions and
stereotype-inconsistent conditions did not differ on either
the positive or negative life-outcomes variables. As such,
all further analyses were conducted weighing Conditions
1 and 4 equivalently (known hereafter as the stereotypeconsistent
conditions), and Conditions 2 and 3 equivalently
(known hereafter as the stereotype-inconsistent
conditions).
Before examining the effects of stereotype condition on
the individual dependent variables, a multivariate analysis
of covariance (MANCOVA) was performed to measure the
impact of stereotype condition on all of the key dependent
variables. The results revealed a marginally significant
effect of condition, F(6, 118)=2.06, p = .06, which was
sufficient to warrant univariate tests. Hence, orthogonal
planned comparisons, weighing the stereotype-consistent
conditions against the stereotype-inconsistent conditions
(+1 −1 −1 +1), were performed on each of the dependent
variables.
Life Outcomes
A planned comparison was conducted to determine
whether the average ratings of positive life outcomes
differed across stereotype conditions. As hypothesized,
this comparison yielded significant results, t(121) = 1.98,
p <.05 with an effect size of η2 =.03, indicating that participants
in the stereotype-inconsistent conditions predicted
more positive life outcomes for themselves than those in the
stereotype-consistent conditions (see Table 1 for means and
standard deviations of all dependent variables). However, a
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations for Dependent Variables by Condition
Information Given about Target Model/Condition
Positive Negative Negated No Info.
(n=32) (n=36) (n=30) (n=28)
Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD
Positive Life Outcomes 6.36 1.10 6.56 .91 6.74 1.07 6.21 .97
Negative Life Outcomes 3.07 1.10 2.87 .95 3.22 1.09 3.31 1.06
Mood −.98 1.50 −.10 2.02 −.47 1.61 −1.46 1.72
Appearance SSES 3.04 .84 3.21 .85 3.21 .66 2.80 .77
Social SSES 3.68 .84 3.96 .71 3.81 .67 3.54 .90
Performance SSES 3.92 .61 3.90 .74 3.86 .67 3.78 .68
similar comparison revealed that the negative life outcomes
ratings did not differ across stereotype conditions, t(121) = .79, ns.
Mood
Consistent with the hypothesis, a planned comparison
contrasting the average mood scores in the stereotypeconsistent
conditions against the average mood scores
in the stereotype-inconsistent conditions revealed a
significant finding, t(121) = 3.17, p <.01 with an effect
size of η2 =.08. Thus, participants who received
stereotype-consistent information about the thin-ideal
target were more likely to report being in a negative mood
than participants who received stereotype-inconsistent
information about the target.
State Self-Esteem
As expected, those who were exposed to stereotypeconsistent
information experienced lower levels of state
self-esteem relative to those who were exposed to
stereotype-inconsistent information.Aplanned comparison
testing whether participants would report different levels
of appearance SSES as a function of condition was significant,
t(121) = 2.16, p <.05 with an effect size of η2 =.04,
as was a similar comparison testing for differences in social
SSES as a function of condition, t(121) = 2.01, p <.05
with an effect size of η2 =.03. However, the effect of condition
on performance SSES was not significant, t(121) = .13, ns.
DISCUSSION
It is generally accepted in the body image literature that
viewing images of thin, attractive females leads women to
feel more negatively about their own bodies. However, the
findings from the current study suggest that body image,
in and of itself, may not hold all of the answers to understanding
why women feel self-dissatisfied after exposure to
a thin model. Rather, the social context within which these
comparisons take place may be an important component
of the self-dissatisfaction, such as the beliefs that women
hold about the lifestyles or personality characteristics of
thin-ideal females.
Consistent with the predictions, confirming or disconfirming
life stereotypes about thin-ideal females
significantly impacted the self-satisfaction of the respondents.
Women who received stereotype-disconfirming
information about the thin target reported more optimistic
future life outcomes for themselves, more positive moods,
and higher appearance and social state self-esteem in
comparison to women who received stereotype-confirming
information about the thin target. Yet, women did not
report significant differences on negative life outcomes or
performance state self-esteem. These results seem reasonable
because respondents typically report low probabilities
of negative life outcomes (Markus&Nurius, 1986), and performance
self-esteem (e.g., levels of ability) may be more
remote from lifestyle and body image concerns.
An alternative explanation for the findings is that women
responded only to the life outcome scripts rather than
the association between life outcomes and thinness. Under
this rationale, however, women should have responded
more negatively in Condition 1, when they read the positive
stereotype-confirming information than in Condition 4,
when they read no lifestyle information. The fact that
women responded similarly in these two conditions suggests
that women attribute positive life circumstances to
thin-ideal females even in the absence of additional cueing
and may experience some self-dissatisfaction as a result of
this association.
In addition, because thinness and attractiveness were
confounded in the images, it is unclear whether thinness
or attractiveness affected women’s self-perceptions. It is
likely, however, that both thinness and attractiveness contributed
to women’s self-evaluation. Because media figures
tend to be facially attractive as well as thin in physique,
the dimensions of thin and attractive may be associated in
women’s minds. Nonetheless, future studies that are interested
in exploring the relative effects of weight and attractiveness
may want to add images of fat and average-weight
women to provide further insights on this topic. Including
fat and average-weight images may also enhance our
understanding of the stereotypes that women hold about
physical appearance and weight, such as beliefs that not
being thin may indeed lead to negative consequences (e.g.,
Quinn & Crocker, 1998).
Although there appears to be a relationship between
stereotype belief and self-satisfaction, several limitations
suggest that some caution should be taken in interpreting
the results. First, the life outcomes scale is a newly
developed measure that has not yet been well validated.
Moreover, the effect size of the positive life outcomes
finding was rather small, as were the effect sizes of the
other significant findings. The lack of validation for the life
outcomes measure and the small effect sizes suggest that
more research on stereotype beliefs and self-satisfaction
is needed before more unequivocal conclusions can be
made.
Second, studies have shown that whereas African American
women tend to experience more weight-related body
satisfaction than European American women (Cash &
Henry, 1995), Asian American and Hispanic American
women resemble European American women in their rates
of body-dissatisfaction (Cash & Henry, 1995; Evans &
McConnell, in press). Because racial differences seem to
influence women’s body satisfaction, it is plausible that race
may have affected the results of the current study. Unfortunately,
because this study lacks an adequate number of
women of color to examine their responses separately from
the responses of European American women, these analyses
were not performed. Future studies may gain valuable
insights into women’s body image dissatisfaction by including
more participants of color.
Third, because the dependent variables were not assessed
prior to stimulus exposure, it is possible that participants
differed on negative feelings prior to the experimental
manipulations. However, random assignment to
the different conditions should have minimized this possibility.
Also, because manipulation checks were not performed,
it cannot be stated with certainty that participants
accepted the lifestyle information that they were given or
if participants compared themselves to the target. Finally,
the presentation of life information about a thin-ideal female
image who is ostensibly “real” may be somewhat removed
from typical exposure to such images in decontextualized
settings (e.g., fashion magazines, commercials).
Future research on this topic should consider adapting the
current experimental paradigm using a variety of media
outlets.
Limitations aside, the findings from this study are valuable
because they bring to light another facet of body
image dissatisfaction. Specifically, this study extends past
research on body image by demonstrating that women’s
motivation to attain a thin-ideal physical appearance may
not stem merely from their body dissatisfaction, but also
from their motivation to attain general life satisfaction. In
order to fully understand the psychology of body image
dissatisfaction, we should move beyond focusing solely on
body-dissatisfaction and move toward a more comprehensive
understanding of physical appearance, including how
body-dissatisfaction takes place within a social and cultural
context.
Initial submission: July 12, 2002
Initial acceptance: October 15, 2002
Final acceptance: December 13, 2002
REFERENCES
Cash, T. F., & Henry, P. E. (1995). Women’s body images: The
results of a national survey in the U.S.A. Sex Roles, 33, 19–
28.
Cattarin, J. A., Thompson, J. K., Thomas, C.,&Williams, R. (2000).
Body image, mood, and televised images of attractiveness:
The role of social comparison. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 220–239.
Evans, P. C., & McConnell, A. R. (in press). Do racial minorities
respond in the same way to mainstream beauty
standards? Social comparison processes in Asian, Black, and White women. Self and Identity.
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect
of experimental presentation of thin media images on body
satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal
of Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16.
Harrison, K. (1997). Does interpersonal attraction to thin media
personalities promote eating disorders? Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 41, 478–500.
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation
of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 895–910.
Hebl, M. R., & Heatherton, T. F. (1998). The stigma of obesity in
women: The difference is black and white. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 417–426.
Korbonits, M., Trainer, P. J., Little, J. A., Edwards, R., Kopelman,
P. G., Besser, G. M., Svec, F., & Grossman, A. B. (1997).
Leptin levels do not change acutely with food administration in normal or obese subjects, but are negatively correlated with pituitary-adrenal activity. Clinical Endocrinology, 46, 751–757.
Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist,41, 954–969.
Myers, P. N., & Biocca, F. A. (1992). The elastic body image: The
effect of television advertising and programming on body
image distortions in young women. Journal of Communication, 42, 108–133.
Quinn, D. M., & Crocker, J. (1998). Vulnerability to the affective
consequences of the stigma of overweight. In J. K. Swim &
C. Stangor (Eds.), Prejudice: The target’s perspective (pp.
125–143). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Silverstein, B., Perdue, L., Peterson, B., & Kelly, E. (1986). The
role of the mass media in promoting a thin standard of bodily attractiveness for women. Sex Roles, 14, 519–523.
Spitzer, B. L., Henderson, K. A., & Zivian, M. T. (1999). Gender
differences in population versus media body sizes:
A comparison over four decades. Sex Roles, 40, 545–
565.
Thornton, B., & Maurice, J. (1997). Physique contrast effect: Adverse
impact of idealized body images for women. Sex Roles,
37, 433–439.
Thornton, B., & Maurice, J. (1999). Physical attractiveness
contrast effect and the moderating influence of selfconsciousness. Sex Roles, 40, 379–392.
Thornton, B., & Moore, S. (1993). Physical attractiveness contrast
effect: Implications for self-esteem and evaluations of the
social self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
474–480.
Copyright © 2003 EBSCO Publishing
[…] Example of Research Journal Assignment […]